Social Behaviors In Newly-Hatched Tortoises

Monday morning, London tube. Standing there, in a crowd of hundreds, your mind goes to your plans for the day, your morning appointments, errands to run. But when Chiara, the childhood friend you havenā€™t met in ten years, walks by, you immediately recognize her. A couple of minutes later, you are telling each other all about your lives, still marveling at the slim chances of meeting after all these years. We recognize someone familiar even if we havenā€™t met them in a while, effortlessly. But where does this ability come from? Did we learn it through continuous social interactions or is it a trait we possess since birth?

Scientists have proven that humans share this ability with many other animals. For instance, some years ago the case of sheep that are able to recognize dozens of faces after 2 years went viral. Of course, being able to recognize familiar individuals is extremely useful, because enable individuals to establish closer relationships with relatives, mates, and friends, or to modulate aggressive behaviors toward strangers and neighbors. The ā€œdear enemyā€ phenomenon has been observed for instance with territorial songbirds: individuals are less aggressive toward familiar territorial neighbors, performing fewer threatening displays and biting less compared to what happens with strangers.

All these examples taken from social species, though, donā€™t help to understand whether the ability to recognize familiar individuals requires the training of repeated social interactions. While social recognition has been considered as an adaptation for social life, this idea has never been formally tested. Thatā€™s why we thought it would be very interesting to check whether social recognition is available to non-social species. Are non-social animals able to recognize familiar conspecifics at the onset of life, when they havenā€™t had repeated interactions with different conspecifics? Are social abilities spontaneously available also to non-social animals? The solution to address this issue (see Versace et al., 2018) was located a few kilometers from the city center at Sperimentarea. This is a field station of the Rovereto Civic Museum (Italy), that is becoming a sanctuary for tortoises, especially those endangered and protected exemplars that the local rangers (Guardia Forestale) find when they get lost from local private gardens.

Tortoises are the perfect subjects to understand the origins of social behavior for many reasons. On the one hand, when housed with other tortoises in captivity, they show the ability to learn from the actions of conspecifics (Wilkinson et al., 2010). This tells us that tortoises possess the ability to interact with social partners, but not whether these abilities are spontaneous (the 1973 Nobel prize Konrad Lorenz would say ā€œinnateā€) or result from learning through repeated social interactions. On the other hand, in the wild, tortoise behavior is solitary, namely, they do not form cohesive social groups, and live mostly on their own except for courtship and matingā€¦ so observing social abilities at the onset of life would show that learning is not necessary to perform specific social behaviors.

 

A tortoise during hatching. Picture by Elisabetta Versace

Of course, when it comes to the actual mating, tortoises get very close, and males might even bite the females to be more effective in courtship. During mating, males produce really loud sounds that are highly appreciated by females (Galeotti et al., 2005). But after this brief social activity, male and female go back to their own lives; the female lays the eggs in the ground and then leaves them for good. About two months later, when the hatchlings emerge from the ground, they are completely self-sufficient and are able to move around just a few hours after birth. So, they can be tested at the onset of life without having had any experience with other conspecifics.

For a couple of days before the deposition of the eggs, tortoises are more active and restless, so, to discover where the eggs were laid, Gionata Stancher (the head of the Zoology section of the Museo Civico Rovereto) spied the females that were about the lay eggs and waited for the female to leave the nest after oviposition. This was a team effort, and the gardeners of SperimentArea (Bruno, Diego and colleagues) kept their eyes well open and alerted Gionata on suspicious laying activities. For some weeks, Gionata and his assistants followed every motion of the tortoises to collect the eggs of two species, Testudo graeca and Testudo marginata, from underground. The eggs were then incubated in individual compartments and after hatching we made sure each of them didnā€™t see any other tortoise than another hatchling that was housed together. So, each tortoise hatchling knew one and only one conspecific that became its familiar conspecific and constituted their only social experience. After a familiarization period of two weeks, during which tortoises shared the same ā€œflat,ā€ the moment of the test arrived. Each tortoise was placed in front of a familiar conspecific or in front of a stranger and let free to move, as shown in this (sped-up ā€” donā€™t worry!) video.

We expected to see differences when we paired two familiar tortoises or two unfamiliar tortoises together only if tortoises were capable of recognizing familiar individuals. To understand whether tortoises treated differently familiar and stranger tortoises, we looked at the average distance kept from the conspecific. While familiar subjects moved randomly in the arena, reaching the distance expected by chance, the stranger condition they first moved closer to the unfamiliar tortoise, as to explore, and then moved significantly away from each other. Moreover, unfamiliar subjects tended to spend more time facing each other in the first part of the test, as if they were more interested in the stranger, before facing the opposite direction.

Since we observed a different behavior in familiar and strange pairs, we showed that exposure to a single conspecific is sufficient to grant individual recognition even in a non-social animal such as a land tortoise, and with very limited social experience. This is the first evidence of spontaneous recognition of familiar conspecific in tortoiseā€™s hatchlings.

A possible explanation for the spontaneous avoidance of unfamiliar tortoises in hatchlings is the need to disperse the clutch. Imagine going to spend your holidays on a sandy beach. You might try to find a nice spot to sunbathe, but if the beach is very crowded, there will be a high level of competition since there will be fewer available spots. Each newborn tortoise will need food, water, and shelter to grow up and survive. Since in the wild, both Testudo marginata and Testudo graeca lay multiple clutches per year and each clutch contains 3-7 eggs (1988; Diaz-Paniagua et al., 1997), without dispersal, hatchlings would saturate the carrying capacity of the environment. ā€œLuckily, we are not as asocial as tortoises, or we wouldnā€™t be here talking,ā€ I said to Chiara, after telling her about our summer research.

PictureĀ by Francesco Rossi
Students and researchers at Sperimentarea (Rovereto Civic Museum Foundation): Martina Micheli Zanotti, Gionata Stancher (Rovereto Civic Museum Foundation), Bettina Maccagnani, Elisabetta Versace (Queen Mary, University of London), Silvia Damini (University of Trento), Francesco Rossi, Sara Peruffo (University of Trento). (Credit:Ā Elisabetta Versace)

 

The gardeners of Sperimentarea Diego (left) and Bruno (right) preparing the experimental setup for testing the tortoises. (Credit: Elisabetta Versace)

Pictures at Sperimentarea (by Elisabetta Versace). Sperimentarea is a didactic and research station managed by the Civic Museum of Rovereto. Gionata Stancher is the head of the museumā€™s zoology department. Local rangers bring to Sperimentarea tortoise exemplars lost from local gardens, to keep them in a protected area (not all animals are tagged with microchips). There, students and researchers study the behavior of different species of land tortoises. At the moment Sperimentarea hosts exemplars of Testudo hermanni, Testudo graeca, Testudo marginata, Testudo horsfieldii.

These findings are described in the article entitledĀ Born to be asocial: newly hatched tortoises avoid unfamiliar individuals, recently published in the journalĀ Animal Behaviour.Ā This work was conducted byĀ ElisabettaĀ Versace from theĀ Queen Mary University of London andĀ the University of Trento,Ā SilviaĀ Damini from the University of Trento andĀ the University of Padua,Ā MatteoĀ Caffini from the University of Trento, andĀ GionataĀ Stancher from the University of Trento andĀ Rovereto Civic Museum Foundation.

References:

  1. Diaz-Paniagua, C., Keller, C., Andreu, A.C., 1997. Hatching success, delay of emergence and hatchling biometry of Testudo graeca in Spain.pdf. Journal of Zoology. 243, 543ā€“553.
  2. Galeotti, P., Sacchi, R., Rosa, D.P., Fasola, M., 2005. Female preference for fast-rate, high-pitched calls in Hermannā€™s tortoises Testudo hermanni. Behavioral Ecology. 16, 301ā€“308.
  3. Hailey, A., Loumbourdis, N.S., 1988. Egg size and shape, clutch dynamics, and reproductive effort in European tortoises. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 66, 1527ā€“1536.
  4. Versace, E., Damini, S., Caffini, M., Stancher, G., 2018. Born to be asocial: newly hatched tortoises avoid unfamiliar individuals. Animal Behaviour. 138.
  5. Wilkinson, A., Kuenstner, K., Mueller, J., Huber, L., 2010. Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). Biology letters. 6, 614ā€“616.

Speak Your Mind!

READ THIS NEXT

Work Productivity Loss In A Warming World:Ā Developed Country Vs Developing Country

As an observed fact, global climatic warming since the Industrial Revolution has caused more frequent heat waves across the world. […]

Realization Of Low-Cost Energy Storage And Conversion Technology: A Promising Air Electrode For Zn Air Battery

As a promising post-lithium-ion battery technology, rechargeable Zn air batteries have attracted intense attention due to their high theoretical energy […]

A Model Of Space-based And Object-centered Attention In The Brain

Shifting attention in space is a fundamental biological function. Without attention, it would be impossible to scan the environment and […]

Science Can Now Make Wood Just As Strong As Steel

When most people think about strong materials they tend to put steel up there; steel is definitely stronger than wood. […]

Black Locust Tree: Types And Pictures

The black locust tree, or Robinia pseudoacacia, is a deciduous tree found throughout the southeastern United States, as well as […]

Growth Performance And Intestinal Morphology Of African Catfish: A Comparison Of Diets

Progressive changes in fish larval gastrointestinal tract are similar in all teleosts and are important in defining proper larval feeding […]

Data-intensive Approaches To Creating Innovation For Sustainable Smart Cities

Located at the complex intersection of economic development and environmental change, cities play a central role in our efforts to […]