ADVERTISEMENT

How Well Do You Know Yourself? Measuring Brain Activity During Self-Recognition

What is the self? This question has been fundamental for many centuries, from Aristotle to Freud. Currently, neuroscientists are interested to know how self-related information, such as our own face and name, is processed in our cognitive system to determine the neural representation of the self in the brain.

Thanks to modern neuroimaging techniques, we are beginning to understand how we recognize ourselves. Several studies have shown that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior superior temporal sulcus, brain areas associated with attentional control, as well as the fusiform gyrus, a region engaged to face perception, are especially active during self-face recognition. Psychological studies, more focused on behavioral data, also have shown interesting results about the singularity of the self; we are faster and more accurate when recognizing our own face compared to other faces in a broad range of contexts.

ADVERTISEMENT

This effect, called self-face advantage or self-bias, has been attributed to attentional mechanisms involved during self-processing. Under this view, seeing oneā€™s own face would cause a similar attentional effect as listening to oneā€™s own name, although self-face has some particularities, such as it cannot be shared with anyone else (except for twins), which makes it a more interesting stimulus to investigate the self. However, it should be taken into consideration that our own face is not only the quintessential representation of our identity but is also the most familiar face to each of us. Therefore, it is unknown whether this advantage in recognition is boosted by attention or due to face familiarity, which leads us to the critical question: Is our own face more than a highly familiar face?

Our study addresses this question investigating the temporal dynamics of self-face recognition. Our hypothesis was that self-face involves distinctive neural processing beginning from an early stage, which would indicate that cognitive mechanisms involved during self-face recognition are linked to the idiosyncratic brain response of the self, not to visual familiarity. To test this idea, we designed an experimental task where 25 healthy participants had to identify facial images of three different identities (one’s own face, a friend’s face, and a stranger’s face) while their brain activity was recorded using a 128 electrode EEG system. We also expected to find the same advantage in self-recognition previously seen in other studies, so we additionally collected participantsā€™ response times.

Our results showed that participants recognized their own face faster than other faces, corroborating the self-face advantage phenomenon. Even more interestingly, self-face had a distinctive neural response as early as 200 milliseconds. At this early stage in face processing, self-face is characterized by a lower neural activity compared to other faces. The dissociation between self-face processing and the processing of other faces (either familiar or unknown) happened before reaching facial identity by our cognitive system, which indicates that self-face advantage is not due to familiarity. Rather, the early distinctive processing of the self indicates that self-face perception benefits from bottom-up processing, which is faster than general facial processing, to access to personal information stored in our memory and then achieve recognition. In other words, our own face needs less attentional resources to be processed, which might facilitate its recognition.

Similar to our own name, our own face seems to possess priority in our cognitive system, possibly by its social and personal relevance. Therefore, our own face should be considered more than a highly familiar face, since it leads to information related to us, that is, to the neural representation of the self, in a unique manner.

ADVERTISEMENT

These findings are described in the article entitled Is your own face more than a highly familiar face? recently published in the journal Biological Psychology.

Comments

READ THIS NEXT

A New Twisting Somersault

The twisting somersault is a visually stunning acrobatic maneuver featured in numerous Olympic sports, such as platform and springboard diving, […]

10 Canadian Provinces And 3 Canadian Territories: Map And List

The 10 Canadian provinces areĀ Alberta, British Columbia, Manitoba, New Brunswick, Newfoundland and Labrador, Nova Scotia, Ontario, Prince Edward Island, Quebec, […]

Atomic Radius Trend

The atomic radius trend describes how the atomic radius changes as you move across the periodic table of the elements. […]

World Health Officials Stunned By US Resistance To Breast Feeding

At the World Health Assembly which took place this spring, a resolution intended to encourage the use of breast feeding […]

Photoexcited Electrons On A Nanochip: Dynamic Electro-Optical Spectroscopy

Electrochemical and optical detections are widely applied in biochemical sensing. Traditionally, electrochemical sensing happens on electrodes, and optical sensing happens […]

Self-powered Paper-based Diagnostics At The Point Of Care Testing

Providing high-quality medical diagnostics in low-resource settings, such as forward-deployed military units, rural areas in Africa, or in the middle […]

Geological Influences On Barrier/Lagoon Behavior At Decade To Century Timescales

Barrier/lagoon systems occur on about 10% of the worldā€™s coast and they constitute around 90% of the US east coast. […]

Science Trends is a popular source of science news and education around the world. We cover everything from solar power cell technology to climate change to cancer research. We help hundreds of thousands of people every month learn about the world we live in and the latest scientific breakthroughs. Want to know more?